• google scholor
  • Views: 3300

  • PDF Downloads: 42

Customer Superstitions in the Accommodation Industry: A Demographic Analysis

Lee Keng Ng1 and Tammy Wee2*

1Hospitality Business Programme, Design and Specialised Businesses Cluster, Singapore Institute of Technology, Singapore .

2School of Tourism and Hospitality Management, American University of Phnom Penh, Cambodia .

Corresponding author Email: tammieweeee@gmail.com


DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.12944/JBSFM.03.01-02.13

This research project focussed on the topic of superstitions which is widely prevalent but under-explored. Specifically, this study examined the effects of demographical variables (namely, gender, age, education, income, and religion) on 18 superstitious behaviours. An online survey was distributed using two non-probability sampling techniques: snowballing and voluntary response sampling. The data collection was done through known contacts and on a social media platform within online communities. The survey was open to participants who were at least 18 years old and fitted the other criterion of being guests at hotels, hostels, Airbnb, etc. A total of 126 usable responses was collected and analysed. Descriptive statistics and MANOVA results indicated that demographical variables did not have much effect on superstitious behaviours. Furthermore, the extent of superstitious behavioural displays was also not varied among respondents with different demographical profiles. In a challenging business landscape, it is relevant and important for the accommodation industry to have heightened awareness about guests superstitions. Such knowledge can aid the operators in exhibiting extra cultural sensitivity and gracious respect towards their guests, generating more satisfaction and building loyalty.

Accommodation; Culture; Demographic; Superstitious Behaviour

Copy the following to cite this article:

Ng L. K, Wee T. (2021) "Customer Superstitions in the Accommodation Industry: A Demographic Analysis". Journal of Business Strategy Finance and Management, 3(1,2). DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.12944/JBSFM.03.01-02.13

Copy the following to cite this URL:

Ng L. K, Wee T. (2021) "Customer Superstitions in the Accommodation Industry: A Demographic Analysis". Journal of Business Strategy Finance and Management, 3(1,2). Available From: https://bit.ly/3ClgrNW


Download article (pdf)
Citation Manager
Publish History


Article Publishing History

Received: 2021-06-30
Accepted: 2021-09-15
Reviewed by: Orcid Orcid Majid Mohammad Shafiee
Second Review by: Orcid Orcid Rashed Al Karim
Final Approval by: Dr. Tahir Sufi


Introduction

It is no overstatement that most people are at least a little superstitious, whether it is placing trust inauspicious numbers, knocking on wood for good luck, or believing that Friday the 13th is an unlucky day (Diamond, 2017; Kramer & Block, 2011). What is superstition? Researchers have different definitions for a concept that involves religion, numerical homonym, traditional customs, feng-shui, good luck charms, etc. (Huang & Teng, 2009; Tobacyk & Milford, 1983; Torgler, 2007).

Superstitions have endured despite all the technological progress and dominance. Even if the superstitious beliefs do not make sense, people persist in letting the irrational beliefs continue to impact their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (University of Chicago Booth School of Business, 2015). Superstitions give meaning to the random nature of luck; they have the appeal as a remedy for people to manage their fears, anxieties, and hope (Hood, 2010; Mulvania, n.d.).

When people travel, they bring their superstitious traditions along with them (McCartney, 2013). The experiences of being in less familiar or unfamiliar environments could induce a fear of uncertainty and unknown risk (Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005; Sun, 2014, Yusof, 2015). Hence, guests observe superstitions to have a sense of security, control, and ease, especially when faced with unpredictable or inexplicable phenomena at the accommodation premises (Thio, 2015; Vaidyanathan et al., 2018).

While superstition has been examined in the contexts of business (Burakov, 2018;   Raesita & Mahadwartha, 2020; Simmons & Robert, 2003; Wu et al., 2016), education (Abitov et al., 2018; Chakraborty, 2017; Undie & Ushie, 2018), and even sports (Gmelch, 1972; Hagan et al., 2019; Jirásek, 2018), there is the very limited academic study of superstitions in the accommodation industry (Pratt & Kwan, 2019). There are only a couple of research papers that discussed how numerological superstitions affect the numbering and assigning of hotel floors and rooms (Pratt & Kirillova, 2019; Pratt & Kwan, 2019). T
here is no scholastic research that explored accommodation guests’ range of superstitious beliefs and behaviors.

Thus, this study was designed to achieve two objectives to fill the gaps in the literature: to examine the variety of superstitions that guests have when they stay at accommodations and to understand how demographics might affect superstitions. The findings have apparent relevance for the accommodation industry’s operators who could use the new knowledge to improve guests’ experience and loyalty by respecting their superstitious beliefs and practices (eHotelier, 2019).

Literature Review

Demographical Variables

Literature suggests that the antecedents of superstitions could include demographic factors such as gender, age, educational level, economic status, and religion (Dag, 1999; Pratt & Kirillova, 2019; Vaidyanathan et al., 2018; Vyse, 2014; Wolfradt, 1997).

Findings suggested that women generally tend to be more superstitious than men, ever since their predominant presence in religious events in feudal times (Valussi, 2020). Women are more probable to demonstrate superstitious behaviors such as wanting to change a 13th-floor hotel room in order to have an inner calm and ‘protect’ themselves (Carroll, 2007; Huang & Teng, 2009; Orenstein, 2002; Tobacyk & Milford, 1983; Wiseman & Watt, 2004; Yang et al., 2018).

Contrary to common guesses, younger people could be more prone to believing in superstitions (Torgler, 2007). Being younger, they could feel less certain about the future; they are more likely to adopt socially accepted behaviors so as to seek a sense of social belongingness (Gere & MacDonald, 2010; Orenstein, 2002; Torgler, 2007; Victor & Yang, 2012). There is also a different view that there might be only a slight variation between the younger and the older individuals. For instance, Carroll’s (2007) study showed that 12% of those aged between 18 to 49 years old, and 14% of those aged 50 years old and older were similarly bothered when assigned the 13th floor of a hotel.

While some researchers found no association between superstition and educational level, other researchers claimed that individuals with a higher level of education are likely to be less superstitious (Aarnio & Linderman, 2005; Barro & McCleary, 2002; Mowen & Carlson, 2003; Salter & Routledge, 1971). This is because higher education typically includes knowledge and academic training about critical, logical thinking. The non-association between superstitions and high educational level could also be a result of social justice since individuals with superstitious behaviors could be perceived by others as being uneducated, irrational, and incompetent (Case et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2015).

It was found that people with relative deprivation (i.e., worse off on some economic standard or lacking advantageous life situations) are more likely to use superstitions to give themselves the perceived control to cope with their higher psychological distress (Abrams et al., 1999; Burger, 1989; Rice, 2003; Smith et al., 2012; Stark & Bainbridge, 1980). It was found that those in the lower social class or who were unemployed were more inclined to believe in the power of horoscopes and lucky charms (Torgler, 2007). For this study, monthly income was chosen as a variable instead of employment that was used by Pratt and Kirivolla (2019) as the respondents who were contacted via the non-probability sampling were most probably employed.

The relationship between religion and superstitions is a non-conclusive one. On one hand, some studies indicated that there is a positive relationship between religion and superstitions because both involve philosophical beliefs that resist the laws of science (Goode, 2000; MacDonald, 1995; Orenstein, 2002; Torgler, 2003). On the other hand, another set of findings suggested there is an inverse relationship between religion and the belief in paranormal phenomena (Ellis, 1988; Rice, 2003). People without religious affiliation were found to be more likely to believe the supernatural as compared to those who have religion. One key reason is that activities involving tarot cards and crystal power are specifically rejected by the church and some other religious establishments (Emmons & Sobal, 1981; Goode, 2000; Zinnbauer et al., 1997).

Types of Superstitions

Whether superstitions are extreme or harmless, they are widely practiced, based on online commentaries and news articles. The more frequently mentioned ones are discussed here and were incorporated into the online survey instrument. To begin with, it is uncommon to use 13 for the designations of rooms or floors in hotels. The elevator buttons may skip from 12 to 14, or 12A is used instead (De Lollis, 2007; eHotelier, 2019; Prakash, 2016). The other number that is avoided is 4 which is deemed ominous because it sounds like ‘death’ in Mandarin and Chinese dialects (Diamond, 2017; Prakash, 2016; Romero, 2016).

One superstition requires guests to knock (up to three times) before entering the rooms.
The knocking is supposed to courteously inform the spirits of guests’ arrival, respectfully ask the spirits to depart, and leave the guests alone (eHotelier, 2019; Thio, 2015; Woon, 2019). Upon room entry, there is a series of superstition-driven behaviors: open all the curtains, open all the windows too, and switch on all the lights (Oei, 2013). All these are to let sunlight and fresh air (all positive energy) into the room and announce the guests’ presence to any spirits (Yusof, 2015). Another belief cum behavior upon room entry is to flush the toilet immediately. This is to flush away any negative energy and once again inform the spirits that someone has arrived (Oei, 2013; Silverman, 2017; Woon, 2019).

During the stay, it
is recommended to place random belongings on all beds, be it luggage, clothes, or toiletries. The other option is to simply mess up the bedding of any unoccupied bed so that the spirits would not claim the vacant bed for their use (eHotelier, 2019; Silverman, 2017; Thio, 2015; Woon, 2019; Yusof, 2015). However, random belongings do not include hats. This is because some think that evil spirits may dwell inside hats (Oei, 2013). It was also said that in Italy, doctors and priests on house calls would drop their hats on the sick or dying person’s bed. Thus, if a hat is placed on the bed, the sleeper will be condemned to bad luck (Marchetti, 2020).

Regarding fittings in the room, there are a few misgivings about mirrors. Some cultures believe that it is bad luck to sleep facing a mirror as the mirror could sap away the positive energy in the room, deflect away good luck, or steal the sleeper’s soul (Oei, 2013; Silverman, 2017; Woon, 2019; Yusof, 2015). There is an accompanying belief that the soul leaves the body at night while the individual is asleep. Hence, the soul may get confused by the reflection in the mirror and fail to return to the body (Thio, 2015).

To augment all the earlier actions of knocking, letting in the light, and flushing the toilet, individuals could take extra measures by patting the pillows before going to bed. This serves to send a repeated signal to the spirits that the bed is in use (Chan, 2020;  Silverman, 2017). Individuals could also place their shoes in opposite directions, with one shoe upside down. This is to confuse the spirits about the owners’ whereabouts; additionally, it makes it harder for any spirits to put on the footwear and take control over the shoe owners body or life (Oei, 2013; Silverman, 2017; Thio, 2015; Woon, 2019; Yusof, 2015).

As for items in the room, it is believed that touching or moving holy books might adversely affect the protective powers (Woon, 2019). Thus, it is recommended to refrain from disturbing the items that are meant to keep the individuals safe (Silverman, 2017). It is also believed that an open umbrella indoors may lead to financial doom due to natural disasters or other misfortune. This could be why hotels choose to have umbrella holders outside their properties, for reasons beyond housekeeping purposes (eHotelier, 2019).

In the Chinese tradition, there are ‘yin’ and ‘yang’ places, referring to the dark and light portions of the universe respectively. As closets are sometimes musty, they are considered as ‘yin’ places where evil spirits may linger in and even wear the clothes (Silverman, 2017; Woon, 2019). Related to the ‘yin’ element, i
t is thought that spirits tend to gather in areas with less human traffic and energy, such as at the end of the hall or near to stairwell. To compensate for the inadequate amount of vitality, individuals who stay in such ‘remote’ rooms ought to make some noise, such as having music to increase the level of liveliness (Thio, 2015).

Methodology

Research Design


Two non-probability sampling techniques were utilized in this research, namely, snowballing and voluntary response sampling. Snowball sampling was selected because the sample that fits the characteristics of the targeted population is known to the researchers, and they are more readily and easily reachable than unknown individuals. This method also allowed the researchers to ensure that the sample possesses the heterogeneity elements of the population. Voluntary response sampling was used, having considered the nature of the study that involved the personal conduct of superstitious behaviors. Hence, individuals could voluntarily decide if they wanted to be included in the sample to indicate their responses.

The distribution of the survey was done in two ways: on a social media platform within online communities and via an online questionnaire platform, Qualtrics. Anyone who is at least 18 years old and fits the other criterion of being a guest at hotels, hostels, Airbnb, etc., could participate in the survey. The survey with an anonymous link was distributed between March and May 2021. The data collected will not be traced back to the respondents and no identifying information was collected.

Questionnaire Design

The questionnaire had three sections. The first section was designed to include some inclusion/exclusion questions with the intention of recruiting the right group of people to maintain data reliability. It comprised the qualifying questions to ensure that the respondents fit the criteria of being guests at hotels, hostels, Airbnb, etc., and are at least 18 years old.

The second section consisted of various superstitions expressed in 18 statements (Table 1) to contextualize superstitious behaviors, habits, and beliefs. The constructs encompassed numerology belief, room attributes, housekeeping nuances, and social practices, which were sourced from various academic journals (e.g., Carlson et al., 2009; Dag, 1999; Delacroix & Guillard, 2008; Pratt & Kirillova, 2019) and informal sources such as websites that document these superstitious behaviors (e.g., Chan, 2020; De Lollis, 2007; Diamond, 2017; eHotelier, 2019; Oei, 2013; Prakash, 2016; Silverman, 2017; Thio, 2015; Woon, 2019).

Table 1: The 18 Statements / Questionnaire Items.

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

Knock on the room door before entering.

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

Not open an umbrella indoors.

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

 

The questionnaire items that operationalized the various constructs were measured on a five-point Likert scale (1=Strongly Disagree and 5=Strongly Agree) in the online survey format. The final section of the questionnaire collected demographical data of gender, age group, educational level, monthly income, and religion. A pre-testing exercise was conducted on three individuals to check for awkward wording, ambiguous questions, leading questions, or compound questions. Some minor re-rephrasing was subsequently done to increase the clarity. The entire survey was done anonymously, with no identifier to link back to the respondents.

Data Analysis

The data set was checked for missing values, outliers, and other statistical issues that might affect the result. Internal consistency was also established, with Cronbach's Alpha score of .935.

To fulfil the first research aim of examining the variety of superstitions of accommodation guests based on their demographic, a descriptive statistic on the demographic was conducted to illustrate this. To achieve the second aim of uncovering the effects of demographics on superstitions, a one-way MANOVA analysis incorporating all 18 dependent variables of superstitious behaviors was carried out with each independent variable (i.e., gender, age, educational level, monthly income, and religion).

Demographic Analysis

The eventual sample size of 126 has the demographics as shown in Table 2.

Table 2: Demographics.

Gender

Female

63% (n = 80)

Male

37% (n = 46)

 

Age

18 - 29 years old

48% (n = 60)

30 - 39 years old

25% (n = 32)

40 - 49 years old

  6% (n = 7)

50 - 59 years old

14% (n = 18)

60 - 69 years old

   6% (n = 8)

70 years old & above

   1% (n = 1)

 

Educational Level

Diploma

  6% (n = 7)

Undergraduate Degree

67% (n = 85)

Master's Degree

15% (n = 19)

Doctoral Degree

   7% (n = 9)

Others

   5% (n = 6)

 

Monthly Income

(SGD = Singapore dollar)

Under SGD$5,000 (Under US$3,800)

63% (n = 79)

SGD $5,001 - $10,000 (US$3,801 - US$7,500)

29% (n = 36)

Above SGD$10,000 (Above US$7,500)

  9% (n = 11)

 

Religion

Buddhism

29% (n = 37)

Christianity

29% (n = 36)

Islam

  3% (n = 4)

No religion

29% (n = 37)

Others

10% (n = 12)

 

Preferred Accommodation

Hotel

87% (n = 110)

Rented one (e.g., Airbnb)

13% (n = 16)

 

The respondents consist of more females (63%, n = 80) than males (37%, n = 46). Nearly half belong to the age group of 18 to 29 years old (48%, n = 60), which explains why 63% (n = 79) of the entire sample have a monthly income of less than SGD$5,000 (US$3,800). The majority of the respondents are well educated, with 89% (n = 113) holding an undergraduate degree or higher academic qualification. As for religion, most of them are either Buddhists (29%, n = 37), Christians (29%, n = 36), or have no religion (29%, n = 37). The preferred accommodation type among the respondents is a hotel (87%, n = 110).

The mean scores show that there is no behavior that both genders are more prone to do when staying at accommodations (Table 3). No statement has a mean score that is above 4 (out of a maximum 5). While male and female respondents are equally inclined to knock on the room door before entering, they are similarly least likely to refrain from placing hats on beds.

Table 3: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) for Superstitious Behaviours based on Gender.

 

Male

Female

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

3.07

3.20

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

2.26

2.36

Knock on the room door before entering.

3.54

3.81

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

2.17

2.30

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

3.33

3.74

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

2.98

3.24

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

2.26

2.57

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

1.96

1.93

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

3.04

3.11

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

1.98

2.25

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

2.24

2.16

Not open an umbrella indoors.

2.80

2.86

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

2.17

2.70

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

2.09

2.40

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

2.17

2.21

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

2.93

2.98

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

2.78

2.66

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

2.00

2.12

 

There is no highlighted similarity or difference in behaviors between the younger and older respondents (Table 4). The only behavior that has a relatively higher mean across all the age groups, especially for those who are 60 to 69 years old, is the switching on of all lights upon room entry. The two behaviors of knocking before entering the room and drawing open the curtains have high scores across the age groups, except for those who are 40 to 49 years old and anyone who is above 70 years old, respectively.

Table 4: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) for Superstitious Behaviours based on Age.

 

18 – 29 yrs old

30 – 39 yrs old

40 – 49 yrs old

50 – 59 yrs old

60 – 69 yrs old

> 70 yrs old

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

3.32

3.22

2.75

3.06

2.5

2.83

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

2.4

2.07

2.50

2.53

2.13

2.17

Knock on the room door before entering.

3.92

3.78

2.75

3.53

3.25

3.83

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

2.00

2.52

2.75

2.53

2.50

1.83

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

3.60

3.52

3.75

3.53

4.00

3.17

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

3.05

3.04

4.13

3.41

3.25

2.33

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

2.47

2.74

2.00

2.59

2.13

1.83

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

1.77

2.26

1.75

2.18

2.00

1.67

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

3.20

3.04

2.75

2.76

3.00

3.67

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

2.15

2.26

1.88

2.06

2.25

2.17

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

2.13

2.33

2.38

2.18

2.38

1.67

Not open an umbrella indoors.

3.13

2.78

2.25

2.65

2.13

2.50

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

2.77

2.52

1.63

2.53

1.75

2.00

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

2.52

2.19

1.88

2.24

1.75

1.83

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

2.27

2.3

1.88

2.06

2.13

2.00

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

3.05

2.85

2.25

3.29

3.25

2.17

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

2.73

2.74

2.5

2.82

2.63

2.33

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

1.93

2.30

1.75

2.29

2.38

2.00

 

The two behaviours of switch on all the lights upon room entry and knocking before entering the room have high scores across the educational levels, except for respondents with a doctoral degree. Those with master’s degrees also tend to do that, in addition to having the highest inclination to believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky. While respondents with a doctoral degree are less prone to doing most of the superstitious behaviors as compared to the other groups with lower educational levels, they are relatively more neutral (and disagree less) about the patting of pillows before going to bed. The one superstitious behavior that is least likely to be done by all respondents across educational levels is covering all mirrors in the room before going to sleep for the night (Table 5).

Table 5: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) for Superstitious Behaviours based on Educational Level.

 

Diploma

Under-
graduate

Master's

Doctoral

Others

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

2.71

3.18

3.53

2.67

2.83

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

2.43

2.28

2.58

2.00

2.50

Knock on the room door before entering.

3.14

3.98

3.26

2.44

4.00

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

2.14

2.27

2.37

1.89

2.33

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

4.14

3.68

3.42

2.44

3.83

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

2.71

3.24

3.32

2.56

2.67

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

2.14

2.60

2.32

2.00

2.00

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

1.71

1.93

2.21

1.67

1.83

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

2.29

3.25

2.74

2.67

3.5

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

1.71

2.25

2.00

2.00

2.00

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

1.71

2.29

2.05

2.11

1.83

Not open an umbrella indoors.

2.57

2.96

2.79

2.11

2.67

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

1.86

2.75

2.00

2.00

2.17

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

1.86

2.51

1.79

1.78

2.00

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

1.71

2.27

2.26

1.67

2.33

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

2.86

3.06

2.95

2.33

2.67

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

2.14

2.71

2.84

2.89

2.67

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

1.86

2.19

1.79

1.78

2.17

 

Those with a monthly income under Singapore $5,000 (US$3,800) are most likely to knock on the room door before entering and switching on all the lights. The next group with a higher monthly income of Singapore 5,0001 to $10,000 (US$3,801 to $7,500) has the same inclinations to perform these two behaviors, albeit with a slightly lower mean score. Those who earn more than Singapore $10,000 (US$7,500) a month are also most likely to knock on the room door before entering. However, this group with the highest monthly income is comparatively less inclined than the other groups in a few behavioral aspects, such as not placing hats on the bed, covering all mirrors in the room before going to bed, or arranging shoes so that one shoe is upside down (Table 6).

Table 6: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) for Superstitious Behaviours based on Monthly Income.

 

< SGD$5,000
(< US$3,800)

SGD$5,001 - SGD$10,000
(US$3,801 - US$7,500)

> $10,000
> US$7,500)

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

3.35

2.87

2.69

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

2.39

2.16

2.31

Knock on the room door before entering.

3.90

3.32

3.56

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

2.30

2.29

1.94

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

3.71

3.45

3.25

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

3.23

3.23

2.56

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

2.67

2.16

2.00

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

2.01

1.84

1.75

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

3.15

3.00

2.94

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

2.18

2.29

1.75

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

2.28

2.16

1.81

Not open an umbrella indoors.

3.14

2.48

2.06

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

2.71

2.16

2.19

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

2.48

2.06

1.75

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

2.39

1.84

1.94

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

3.11

2.74

2.63

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

2.91

2.29

2.5

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

2.11

1.94

2.19

 

For all religious groups, they are more prone to switch on all the lights upon room entry, among the 18 behaviors. For respondents who choose Buddhism or other religions, they have a high tendency to knock on the room door before entering, with mean scores of 4.41 and 4.25, respectively. Even those who have no religion show inclination to knock on the door before entry and turn on all lights after entry. As compared to respondents with other religious affiliations, the respondents who choose Islam are relatively more likely to turn on all the lights; they are also relatively less likely to perform 15 out of 18 behaviors, especially arranging shoes to point in different directions that has a mean score of 1 (Table 7).

Table 7: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) for Superstitious Behaviours based on Religion.

 

Buddhism

Christianity

Islam

No religion

Others

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

3.59

2.64

2.00

3.3

3.25

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

2.57

1.94

1.50

2.41

2.75

Knock on the room door before entering.

4.41

2.86

2.25

3.84

4.25

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

2.49

1.89

3.00

2.24

2.42

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

3.84

3.39

4.25

3.38

3.83

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

3.57

2.94

3.50

2.84

3.25

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

2.81

2.08

1.75

2.32

3.17

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

2.22

1.67

1.25

1.89

2.25

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

3.43

2.75

2.25

2.95

3.75

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

2.51

1.89

2.00

2.03

2.25

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

2.49

1.92

1.50

2.16

2.42

Not open an umbrella indoors.

3.57

2.50

1.25

2.54

3.08

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

3.30

1.75

1.00

2.38

3.25

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

2.86

1.61

1.25

2.27

2.92

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

2.62

1.81

1.50

2.05

2.75

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

3.51

2.56

1.75

2.81

3.33

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

3.14

2.33

2.25

2.57

3.08

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

2.43

1.78

1.25

2.05

2.25

 

As a summary, the two behaviors of knocking on the room door before entering and turning on all lights after entering have a mean score of more than 3 for all demographical variables (Table 8). The demographical variable of gender has more affirmative responses to more behavioral statements, including the belief in a lucky or unlucky number and not sleeping in a room with the mirror facing the bed. The demographical variable of age also shows responses to behaviors such as drawing open all curtains upon room entry. The majority of the mean scores are less than 3, implying that the sample in this study is generally not prone to displaying most of the superstitious behaviors.

Table 8: Mean Scores (Maximum 5) of Superstitious Behaviours based on Demographical Variables.

 

Gender

Age

Educational Level

Monthly Income

Religion

Believe that certain numbers are lucky or unlucky.

> 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Refuse to stay on a floor associated with an “unlucky” number.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Knock on the room door before entering.

> 3

> 3

> 3

> 3

> 3

Upon entering the room, open all the windows.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Upon entering the room, switch on all the lights.

> 3

> 3

> 3

> 3

> 3

Upon entering the room, draw open all the curtains.

< 3

> 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Place random belongings or anything to occupy the unused bed.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Avoid placing hats on the bed.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Avoid sleeping in a room with a mirror facing the bed.

> 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Cover all mirrors in the room before going to bed.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Make some noise if staying in a room located at the end of the hall or near the stairwell.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Not open an umbrella indoors.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Arrange shoes to point in different directions.

< 3

< 3

< 3

<3

< 3

Arrange shoes so that one shoe is upside down.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Change a room if the Bible or any Holy scripture has been left open upon check-in.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Flush the toilet before the first usage.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Pat the pillow before going to bed.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

Not hang up clothes in the closet.

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

< 3

 

To further confirm how demographics might have affected superstitions, a one-way MANOVA analysis was conducted. The results indicated no statistically significant difference in superstitious behaviors for all variables.

For demographical variables such as gender, age, education, income, and religion, the p-value is more than .05; Wilk's Λ for gender = 0.887, age = 0.408, educational level = 0.538, monthly income = 0.687, and religion = 0.478 respectively. The partial η2  ranges between .113 to .171, which again indicated that the results lack any significant effects. In other words, none of the five demographical variables affects superstitious behaviors (Table 9).

Table 9: One-Way MANOVA for Demographics.

 

Effect

Value

Significance

Partial η2

Gender

Wilks' Lambda

0.887

0.740

0.113

Age

Wilks' Lambda

0.408

0.201

0.164

Educational level

Wilks' Lambda

0.538

0.538

0.144

Monthly Income

Wilks' Lambda

0.687

0.198

0.171

Religion

Wilks' Lambda

0.478

0.165

0.169

 

Discussion

Past research highlighted that gender, age, education, economic status, and religion could affect superstitious behaviors. However, the results from this study reveal that demographical variables do not have much effect on superstitious behaviors. There are only minimal differences in certain behaviors observed across the demographical variables, such as the knocking on the room door and the switching on of all lights. This suggests that the respondents may still be somewhat superstitious but to a small extent. If demographics do not impact superstitious behaviors, then what could be some other influencing factors? There were suggestions that superstitious behaviors might be due to individuals’ acquiescence to follow common intuitions (Risen, 2016) or the desire to have a sense of control over phenomena (Dagnall & Drinkwater, 2018).

Notwithstanding the results from this study, it is still commercially warranting for accommodation operators to observe, respect, and respond to any of their guests’ superstitions. For example, operators could plan room design to include a master switch at the door entrance to turn on all the lights or avoid placing a mirror facing the bed. After all, respecting guests’ subtle superstitious inclinations as part of service and customer value creation.

For future research, a different sampling could perhaps be adopted. This study did not follow the basic tenets of inferential statistical analyses and had instead employed the usage of a non-probability sampling method (purpose sampling). For future studies, the use of probability sampling from a target population (for instance, hotel guest lists) could reflect a different set of insights into this topic of superstitious behaviors.

Alternatively, a broader measure of superstition types that includes more forms and diversity could be another direction for future research. For example, region-specific superstitions could be explored so as to increase the applicability of results to a particular geographical or cultural setting. In addition, psychological aspects (such as the sense of control that is mentioned earlier in the discussion section) are other measures that could be examined.

Conclusion

Superstitions are part of any culture, all over the world. Despite their prevalence, there is little research that was conducted on this topic for the accommodation industry sector. It is an area of study that could be investigated more systematically as accommodation operators have to be culturally sensitive and respectful of guests’ personal beliefs and behaviors. Meeting guests’ superstition-related requests is simply a part of service management.

Acknowledgment

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Funding Source

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of Interest

The authors reported no potential competing interests.

References

  1. Aarnio, K., & Lindeman, M. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, education, and thinking styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1227 -1236. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.009.
    CrossRef
  2. Abitov, I., Gorodetskaya, I., Akbirova, R., & Sibgatullina, L. (2018). Superstitiousness and paranormal beliefs of engineering students comparing to students majoring in sciences, arts and humanities. Espacios, 39(10), 28.
    CrossRef
  3. Abrams, D., Hinkle, S., & Tomlins, M. (1999). Leaving Hong Kong? The roles of attitude, subjective norm, perceived control, social identity and relative deprivation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23(2), 319 -338. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(98)00041-8.
    CrossRef
  4. Barro, R. J., & McCleary, R. M. (2002). Religion and Political Economy in an International Panel. NBER Working Papers 8931, National Bureau of Economic Research, Inc. https://doi.org/10.3386/w8931.
    CrossRef
  5. Burakov, D. (2018). Do discounts mitigate numerological superstitions? Evidence from the Russian real estate market. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(5), 467 -470. https://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:jdm:journl:v:13:y:2018:i:5:p:467-470.
  6. Burger, J. M. (1989). Negative reactions to increases in perceived personal control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 246 – 256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.246.
    CrossRef
  7. Carlson, B., Mowen, J., & Fang, X. (2009). Trait superstition and consumer behavior: re-conceptualisation, measurement, and initial investigations. Psychology and Marketing, 26(8), 689 – 713. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20295.
    CrossRef
  8. Carroll, J. (2007). Thirteen percent of Americans bothered to stay on hotels' 13th floor. https://news.gallup.com/poll/26887/thirteen-percent-americans-bothered-stay-hotels-13th-floor.aspx.
  9. Case, T. I., Fitness, J., Cairns, D. R., & Stevenson, R. J. (2004). Coping with uncertainty: superstitious strategies and secondary control. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(4), 848 – 871. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02574.x.
    CrossRef
  10. Chakraborty, S. (2017). A comparative analysis of superstitious beliefs existing among college students. International Journal of Educational Science and Research, 7(6), 57 – 66. https://doi.org/10.24247/ijesrdec20177.
    CrossRef
  11. Chan, M. (2020). 13 weird travel superstitions from all over the world. https://explore.traveloka.com/features/weird-travel-superstitions.
  12. Dag, I. (1999). The relationships among paranormal beliefs, locus of control and psychopathology in a Turkish college sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 723 – 737. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00184-6.
    CrossRef
  13. Dagnall, N., & Drinkwater, K. (June 2, 2018). The science of superstition – and why people believe in the unbelievable. https://theconversation.com/the-science-of-superstition-and-why-people-believe-in-the-unbelievable-97043.
  14. Delacroix, E., & Guillard, V. (2008). Understanding, defining and measuring the trait of superstition, Selected Proceedings of the IAREP/SABE 2008 Conference at LUISS in Rome, Luiss University Press: Rome.  https://www.researchgate.net/publication/41221415_Understanding_defining_and_measuring_the_trait_of_superstition.
  15. De Lollis, B. (2007). Some hotels now include floor no. 13. Hartford Business. https://www.hartfordbusiness.com/article/some-hotels-now-include-floor-no-13.
  16. Diamond, M. (2017). 20 bizarre superstitions from around the world and the meaning behind them. https://www.businessinsider.com/superstitions-around-the-world-2017-9.
  17. Hotelier (2019). Top 5 weird superstitions hotels have to deal with. https://insights.ehotelier.com/insights.
  18. Ellis, L. (1988). Religiosity and superstition: Are they related or separate phenomena? Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 25(2), 12 – 13.
  19. Emmons, C. F., & Sobal, J. (1981). Paranormal beliefs: testing the marginality hypothesis. Sociological Focus, 14(1), 49 – 56). https://doi.org/10.1080/00380237.1981.10570381.
    CrossRef
  20. Gere, J., & Macdonald, G. (2010). An update of the empirical case for the need to belong. Journal of Individual Psychology, 66(1), 93 – 115.
  21. Gmelch, G. (1972). Baseball in Professional Baseball. In G. P. Stone (Ed.),Games, Sport and Power, Transaction (pp. 128 -137). Dutton.
  22. Goode, E. (2000). Two paranormalisms or two and a half? An empirical exploration. Skeptical Inquirer, 24(1), 29 – 35. https://cdn.centerforinquiry.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/29/2000/01/22164900/p29.pdf.
  23. Hagan, J. E., Schack, T., & Schinke, R. (2019). Sport psychology practice in Africa: Do culture-specific religion and spirituality matter? Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 6(3). https://doi.org/10.14738/assrj.63.6209.
    CrossRef
  24. Hood, B. (2010). The Science of Superstition: How the Developing Brain Creates Supernatural Beliefs. HarperCollins.
  25. Huang, L., & Teng, C. (2009). Development of a Chinese superstitious belief scale. Psychological reports, 104, 807 – 819. https://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.104.3.807-819.
    CrossRef
  26. Jirásek, I. (2018). Religion and Spirituality in Sport. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.149.
    CrossRef
  27. Kramer, T., & Block, L. (2011). Nonconscious effects of peculiar beliefs on consumer psychology and choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 101 – 111.
    CrossRef
  28. MacDonald, W. (1995). The effects of religiosity and structural strain on reported paranormal experiences. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 34(3), 366 – 376.
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1386885.
    CrossRef
  29. Marchetti, S. (2020). 10 ways to jinx yourself in Italy (and the one way to reverse it). https://www.fodors.com/world/europe/italy/experiences/news/10-ways-to-jinx-yourself-in-italy-and-the-one-way-to-reverse-it.
  30. McCartney, S. (2013). The oddest airline superstitions. https://www.wsj.com/articles/the-oddest-airline-superstitions-1380754239.
  31. Mowen, J. C., & Carlson, B. (2003). Exploring the antecedents and consumer behavior consequences of the trait of superstition. Psychology & Marketing, 20, 1045 -1065. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.10108.
    CrossRef
  32. Mulvania, A. (n.d.). Google Arts & Culture. 18 superstitions from around the world. https://artsandculture.google.com/story/18-superstitions-from-around-the-world/QQIyTWmzJ9QvLg.
  33. Oei, M. (2013). Do you believe in any of these 9 hotel superstitions? https://says.com/my/lifestyle/hotel-room-superstitions.
  34. Orenstein, A. (2002). Religion and paranormal belief. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41(2), 301 – 311. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5906.00118.
    CrossRef
  35. Prakash, A. (2016). The Most Bizarre Travel Superstitions Around The World.  https://www.news.com.au/travel/travel-updates/travel-stories/the-most-bizarre-travel-superstitions-around-the-world/news- story/efab11d306dc556d9894f1c6e6192cf1.
  36. Pratt, S., & Kwan, P. (2019). Unlucky for some? Are some hotel rooms and floors really unluckier than others? International Journal of Culture, Tourism, and Hospitality Research, 13(1), 70 – 83. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCTHR-06-2018-0082.
    CrossRef
  37. Pratt, S., & Kirillova, K. (2019). Are hotel guests bothered by unlucky floor or room assignments? International Journal of Hospitality Management, 83, 83 – 94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2019.03.024.
    CrossRef
  38. Raesita, K., & Mahadwartha, P. A. (2020). Superstitions and Price Clustering in the Taiwan Stock Exchange. 115(Insyma), 483 – 488. https://doi.org/10.2991/aebmr.k.200127.097.
    CrossRef
  39. Reisinger, Y., & Mavondo, F. T. (2005). Travel anxiety and intentions to travel internationally: implications of risk perception. Journal of Travel Research, 43(3), 212 – 225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0047287504272017.
    CrossRef
  40. Rice, T. W. (2003). Believe it or not: religious and other paranormal beliefs in the United States. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, 95 – 106. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5906.00163.
    CrossRef
  41. Risen, J. L. (2016). Believing what we do not believe: Acquiescence to superstitious beliefs and other powerful intuitions. Psychological review, 123(2), 182 – 207. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000017.
    CrossRef
  42. Romero, K. (2016). From avoiding the 4th floor to sacrificing a goat: travel superstitions around the world. https://www.express.co.uk/travel/articles/634254/Travel-superstitions-around-the-world-pictures.
  43. Salter, C. A., & Routledge, L. M. (1971). Supernatural beliefs among graduate students at the University of Pennsylvania. Nature 232, 278 – 279. https://doi.org/10.1038/232278a0.
    CrossRef
  44. Silverman, S. (10 June 2017). 10 Chinese Travel Superstitions. https://www.digmandarin.com/chinese-travel-superstitions.html.
  45. Simmons, L., & Robert, S. (2003). Cultural superstitions and price-endings. Journal of International Marketing, 11(2), 101-111. http://www.jstor.org/stable/25048935.
    CrossRef
  46. Smith, H. J., Pettigrew, T. F., Pippin, G. M., & Bialosiewicz, S. (2012). Relative deprivation: a theoretical and meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(3), 203 – 232. https://doi.org /10.1177/1088868311430825.
    CrossRef
  47. Stark, R., & Bainbridge, W. S. (1980). Networks of faith: interpersonal bonds and recruitment to cults and sects. American Journal of Sociology, 85(6), 1376 – 1395. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2778383.
    CrossRef
  48. Sun, J. (2014). How risky are services? An empirical investigation on the antecedents and consequences of perceived risk for hotel service. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 37, 171 – 179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2013.11.008.
    CrossRef
  49. Thio, B. (2015). 6 bizarre hotel superstitions you’ll pick up while traveling. https://roamscapes.com/crazy-hotel-superstitions.
  50. Tobacyk, J., & Milford, G. (1983). Belief in paranormal phenomena: assessment instrument development and implications for personality functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(5), 1029 – 1037. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.1029.
    CrossRef
  51. Torgler, B. (2003). It Is About Believing: Superstition and Religiosity. CREMA Working Paper Series 2003-10, Center for Research in Economics, Management and the Arts (CREMA).
  52. Torgler, B. (2007). Determinants of superstition. Journal of Socio-Economics, 36(5), 713 – 733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2007.01.007.
    CrossRef
  53. Undie, U. J., & Ushie, B. A. (2018). Influence of sex and superstitious beliefs on Pupils academic performance in the early years’ science in south eastern Nigeria. International Journal of Education and Evaluation, 4(3), 45 – 55. ISSN 2489-0073.
  54. University of Chicago Booth School of Business (2015), The power of magical  thinking: Why superstitions are hard to shake. https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151109160450.htm.
  55. Valussi, E. (2020). Men built religion, and women made it superstitious: gender and superstition in Republican China. Journal of Chinese Religions, 48(1), 87 – 125. https://www.muse.jhu.edu/article/754049.
  56. Vaidyanathan, R., Aggarwal, P., & Bakpayev, M. (2018). A functional motivational framework for examining superstitious behavior. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 3(4), 454 465.
    CrossRef
  57. Victor, C. R., & Yang, K. (2012). The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. Journal of Psychology, 146(1-2), 85 – 104. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2011.613875.
    CrossRef
  58. Vyse, S. (2014). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition. Oxford University Press.
  59. Wang, D., Oppewal, H., & Thomas, D. (2014). Exploring attitudes and affiliation intentions toward consumers who engage in socially shared superstitious behaviors: a study of students in the east and the west. Psychology and Marketing, 31(3), 203 –213. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20687.
    CrossRef
  60. Wiseman, R., & Watt, C. (2004). Measuring superstitious belief: Why lucky charms matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(8), 1533 – 1541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.009.
    CrossRef
  61. Wolfradt, U. (1997). Dissociative experiences, trait anxiety and paranormal beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(1), 15 – 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00043-3.
    CrossRef
  62. Woon, G. (2019). 8 ‘travel superstitions’ your Asian mom probably warned you about. https://www.tripzilla.com/8-travel-superstitions-your-asian-mom-probably-warned-you-about/97532.
  63. Wu, W. T., Chen, C. B., & Chang, C. C. (2016). While number superstitions exist: the influence of prices on mobile phone consumers’ purchase intentions. Journal of Mass Communication and Journalism, 6(3), 1 – 5.  https://doi:10.4172/2165-7912.1000304.
    CrossRef
  64. Yang, E. C. L., Khoo-Lattimore, C., & Arcodia, C. (2018). Power and empowerment: how Asian solo female travellers perceive and negotiate risks. Tourism Management, 68, 32 – 45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2018.02.017.
    CrossRef
  65. Yusof, S. (2015). Secret superstitions: things the hotel staff won’t tell you. https://wanderluxe.theluxenomad.com/things-the-hotel-staff-wont-tell-you.
  66. Zinnbauer, B., Pargament, K., Cole, B., Rye, M., Butter, E., Belavich, T., . . . Kadar, J. (1997). Religion and spirituality: unfuzzying the fuzzy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36(4), 549 – 564. https://doi.org/10.2307/1387689.
    CrossRef
Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.